What Is Ecological Fallacy? | Definition & Example

An ecological fallacy is a logical error that occurs when the characteristics of a group are attributed to an individual. In other words, ecological fallacies assume what is true for a population is true for the individual members of that population.

Example: Ecological fallacy
You are reading a news story about the wealthiest states in the country. Assuming that wealthier states contain more wealthy people is an ecological fallacy. Indeed, it could be due to the fact that they contain a small number of extremely rich individuals.

Ecological fallacy can be problematic for any research study that uses group data to make inferences about individuals. It has implications in fields such as criminology, epidemiology, and economics.

Make your writing flawless in 1 upload

Be assured that you'll submit flawless writing. Upload your document to correct all your mistakes.

upload-your-document-ai-proofreader

What is ecological fallacy?

An ecological fallacy is an error in reasoning. Here, a fallacy is a deduction error, or a mistake we make when moving from the general to the specific. The word ecological is used to refer to a group or system, something that is larger than an individual.

Ecological fallacies occur when we try to draw conclusions about individuals based on data collected at the group level. For example, if a specific neighborhood has a high crime rate, one might assume that any resident living in that area is more likely to commit a crime. Stereotypical thinking like this assumes that groups are homogeneous, while in reality individuals may not necessarily share characteristics of the group they belong to.

The only proofreading tool specialized in correcting academic writing

The academic proofreading tool has been trained on 1000s of academic texts and by native English editors. Making it the most accurate and reliable proofreading tool for students.

Upload my document

When does an ecological fallacy occur?

An ecological fallacy occurs in research designs that use group-level or aggregate-level data to establish whether there is a potential association between two variables. These studies are called ecological studies, a type of observational study where at least one variable is measured at the group level. This can be, for instance, be at a district, state, or country level. Public health research often deals with these types of variables.

Example: Ecological studies and group-level variables
An ecological study that examines the potential association between diet and prostate cancer in different countries may include data such as per-capita sugar consumption and mortality rates.

Per-capita sugar consumption is a variable measured at the group level, because it calculates the average sugar consumption for everyone in the country. Note that it does not mean that every person in the country ate exactly the same amount of sugar! An individual may consume less sugar or more sugar than the average. Similarly, the mortality rate is a group-level variable because it represents what is true for the country, not any individual person’s experience.

Crime rates in a certain area, per-capita sugar consumption, or mortality rates in a country are group-level data. With this data, we can draw conclusions about an area or a country, but not at the individual level. When we are moving from one unit of analysis to another, (i.e., from country level to individual level data), we are committing an ecological fallacy. If we want to draw conclusions about individuals, we need to collect data at an individual level.

What causes an ecological fallacy?

The root cause of ecological fallacies is the misinterpretation of statistical information. Researchers gather statistical data with the aim to generalize from the sample to the population, i.e., from the individual to the population, and not the other way around. When we collect group-level data, it’s a process similar to writing up a summary– certain details of information will be lost or hidden.

For example, in the previous study on prostate cancer, researchers found that there is a correlation between high sugar, meat consumption, and mortality from prostate cancer. Does this mean that we conclude that overindulging in sugar and steaks causes prostate cancer death? Can we use the results as dietary recommendations? The answer is no. While the study does provide insights into risk factors for prostate cancer, it does not establish causality. The data from this study is aggregated:what may apply on a population basis may not necessarily be observed on an individual basis.

Overall, a correlation tends to be larger when an association is assessed at the group level than when it is assessed at the individual level. As a result, when studies like these are analyzed at the individual level, the relationship often disappears.

In other words, while it may be true that countries with higher levels of  sugar consumption have higher rates of prostate cancer deaths, this does not mean that individuals who eat a diet high in  sugar are more likely to die from prostate cancer.

Ecological fallacy example

Ecological fallacies assume that individual members of a group have the average characteristics of the group at large.

Example: Ecological fallacy, average, and median
A primary school teacher wants to see how strong their students are in math. They measure the math scores of two groups, and find that students from Group A score higher on average than students from Group B.

After hearing this, one of the kids’ grandparents proudly looks at their grandchild, who is in Group A, and thinks to themselves “my grandchild must be a math whiz!” However, this is not necessarily true. Just because a student comes from the class with the highest average doesn’t tell us anything about their individual score. The student, for instance, could be the lowest math scorer in a class that otherwise consists of math geniuses.

Assuming that a randomly selected student from Group A would have a better score than a randomly selected student from Group B would be an ecological fallacy. The scores were an average and not a median, so we have no information about the distribution of the scores in the two groups. It might as well be that a randomly selected student from Group B scored higher than one from Group A.

Ecological fallacy example

The only proofreading tool specialized in correcting academic writing

The academic proofreading tool has been trained on 1000s of academic texts and by native English editors. Making it the most accurate and reliable proofreading tool for students.

Upload my document

How to avoid ecological fallacy?

We can avoid ecological fallacies in our own research designs or when interpreting research results from others by following the steps below:

  • Clearly define the unit of analysis. Before collecting your data, consider who or what you are going to analyse. Is it individuals, groups, photos, or social interactions? For example, if you compare the students in two classrooms on test scores, the individual student is the unit. On the other hand, if you want to compare average classroom performance, the unit of analysis is the group, because the data you are analyzing refers to the group, not the individual.
  • Be mindful of logical leaps. When drawing conclusions from your own research, reading research articles, or consuming data-driven news stories, take a minute to think: is the claim at the same level as the data? Or is the claim about individuals, while the data refers to a population? If so, this is a case of ecological fallacy.
  • Keep in mind that results from group-level data can’t be applied to individuals. If you want to investigate individuals or subpopulations within a larger population, make sure that you obtain data from those individuals or subpopulations. This means that you need to follow a suitable sampling method, such as stratified sampling, if you are interested, for example, in specific subpopulations.

Frequently asked questions

What are common types of fallacy in research?

A fallacy is a mistaken belief, particularly one based on unsound arguments or one that lacks the evidence to support it. Common types of fallacy that may compromise the quality of your research are:

  • Correlation/causation fallacy: Claiming that two events that occur together have a cause-and-effect relationship even though this can’t be proven
  • Ecological fallacy: Making inferences about the nature of individuals based on aggregate data for the group
  • The sunk cost fallacy: Following through on a project or decision because we have already invested time, effort, or money into it, even if the current costs outweigh the benefits
  • The base-rate fallacy: Ignoring base-rate or statistically significant information, such as sample size or the relative frequency of an event, in favor of  less relevant information e.g., pertaining to a single case, or a small number of cases
  • The planning fallacy: Underestimating the time needed to complete a future task, even when we know that similar tasks in the past have taken longer than planned
How do you identify an ecological fallacy?

An ecological fallacy can be identified in a study by the following characteristics:

  • Data is obtained at group-level, e.g., country or state level.
  • Data is inferred to individuals
What is an example of ecological fallacy in epidemiology?

An example of ecological fallacy in epidemiology is the relationship between fat consumption and breast cancer.

Studies at the population level have found that there is a correlation between higher rates of fat consumption and higher rates of breast cancer. These findings though have been misinterpreted and some people may think that individuals who consume fat are more likely to develop breast cancer. However, this is not true. A correlation at group level doesn’t necessarily mean that there is also a correlation at individual level. It also doesn’t prove a causal relationship between a high fat diet and breast cancer. Drawing conclusions about individuals based only on analyses of group data is an ecological fallacy.

Sources for this article

We strongly encourage students to use sources in their work. You can cite our article (APA Style) or take a deep dive into the articles below.

This Scribbr article

Nikolopoulou, K. (2023, December 11). What Is Ecological Fallacy? | Definition & Example. Scribbr. Retrieved 9 December 2024, from https://www.scribbr.co.uk/fallacy/the-ecological-fallacy/

Sources

Bell, K. (2020, June 27). ecological fallacy. Open Education Sociology Dictionary. https://sociologydictionary.org/ecological-fallacy/

Colli, J. L., & Colli, A. (2006). International comparisons of prostate cancer mortality rates with dietary practices and sunlight levels. Urologic Oncology: Seminars and Original Investigations, 24(3), 184–194. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.urolonc.2005.05.023

Hsieh, J. J. (2016, August 4). ecological fallacy | epidemiology. Encyclopedia Britannica. https://www.britannica.com/science/ecological-fallacy

Nickerson, C. (2022, May 12). Ecological Fallacy: Definition & Examples. https://www.simplypsychology.org/ecological-fallacy.html

Is this article helpful?
Kassiani Nikolopoulou

Kassiani has an academic background in Communication, Bioeconomy and Circular Economy. As a former journalist she enjoys turning complex scientific information into easily accessible articles to help students. She specialises in writing about research methods and research bias.